DOI: 10.14704/nq.2018.16.9.1537

Towards a Coherent Application of the Beck-Eccles Quantum Trigger

Paul Bessler

Abstract


Prior to the 20th century, the strictly deterministic laws of classical Newtonian physics made the integration of a non-physical entity such as volition into a scientific model of brain dynamics a seemingly impossible task. However, the probabilistic laws of quantum physics discovered in the 1920’s have provided researchers with a valuable new tool, allowing them to account for non-physical entities like volition while still maintaining scientific rigor. One of the greatest challenges that researchers in this field have faced is the application of the atomic-level laws of quantum physics to the study of macroscopic objects. This paper contributes to this line of research by examining the Beck-Eccles quantum trigger model from a neuropsychological processing perspective. Specifically, experimental results from several recent studies of the effects of mindfulness meditation on attentional skills are considered. These results strongly suggest that volition can be used to induce significant changes in the brain’s ability to maintain focus on a single stimulus. In addition, it is shown that the gradual nature of the induced changes is very much consistent with the contention that the Beck-Eccles trigger functions only at an atomic level, and that nonlinear dynamics, and more specifically the principle of self-organization, are needed to apply the trigger’s effects coherently on a macroscopic level.

Keywords


quantum trigger; coherence; volition; mindfulness

Full Text:

PDF

References


Allen M, Dietz M, Blair K, van Beek M, Rees G, Vestergaard-Poulsen P, Lutz A, Roepstorff A. Cognitive-Affective Neural Plasticity following Active-Controlled Mindfulness Intervention. Journal of Neuroscience 2012; 32(44): 15601-10.

Atmanspacher H. Quantum Approaches to Consciousness. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2015/entries/qt-consciousness/ Accessed date: June 21, 2017.

Beck F, Eccles JC. Quantum aspects of brain activity and the role of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 1992; 89: 11357-61.

Beck F. Synaptic Quantum Tunnelling in Brain Activity. NeuroQuantology 2008; 6(2): 140-51.

Brefczynski-Lewis J, Lutz A, Schaefer H, Levinson D, Davidson R. Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 2007; 104(27): 11483-88.

Ciaramelli E, Grady C, Levine B, Ween J, Moscovitch M. Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory are dissociated in posterior parietal cortex: Neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence. Journal of Neuroscience 2010; 30(14): 4943-56.

Descartes R. Discours de la méthode, Paris: Larousse, 1637 (reprint 1934).

Diamond A. Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 2013; 64: 135-68.

Eccles JC. Do mental events cause neural events analogously to the probability fields of quantum mechanics? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 1986; 227(1249): 411-28.

Garrison K, Zeffiro T, Scheinost D, Constable R, Brewer J. Meditation leads to reduced default mode network activity beyond an active task. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 2015; 15(3): 712-20.

Georgiev D, Glazebrook J. Quantum Interactive Dualism: From beck and eccles tunneling model of exocytosis to molecular biology of SNARE zipping. Biomedical Reviews 2014; 25: 15-24.

Hazy T, Frank J, O’Reilly R. Towards an executive without a homunculus: computational models of the prefrontal cortex/basal ganglia system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 2007; 362: 1601-13.

Kabat-Zinn J, Massion AO, Kristeller J, Peterson LG, Fletcher KE, Pbert L, Lenderking WR, Santorelli SF. Effectiveness of a meditation-based stress reduction program in the treatment of anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 1992; 149(7): 936-43.

Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 2003; 10: 144–56.

Katsuki F, Constantinidis C. Bottom-up and top-down attention: different processes and overlapping neural systems. Neuroscientist 2014; 20(5): 509-21.

Libet B. Do we have free will? Journal of Consciousness Studies 1999; 6(8-9): 47-57.

Lutz A, Slagter H, Rawlings N, Francis A, Greischar L, Davidson R. Mental Training Enhances Attentional Stability: Neural and Behavioral Evidence. Journal of Neuroscience 2009, 29(42): 13418-27.

Mak LE, Minuzzi L, MacQueen G, Hall G, Kennedy SH, Milev R. The Default Mode Network in Healthy Individuals: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Brain Connectivity 2017; 7(1): 25-33.

Raymond JE, Shapiro KL, Arnell KM. Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance 1992; 18(3): 849-60.

Stroop J. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 1935; 18(6): 643-62.

Teasdale JD, Segal ZV, Williams JM, Ridgeway VA, Soulsby JM, Lau MA. Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 2000; 68(4): 615-23.

Tiitinen H, Sinkkonen J, Reinikainen K, Alho K, Lavikainen J, Näätänen R. Selective attention enhances the auditory 40-Hz transient response in humans. Nature 1993; 364(6432): 59-60.


Supporting Agencies





| NeuroScience + QuantumPhysics> NeuroQuantology :: Copyright 2001-2018